Conexiant
Login
  • Corneal Physician
  • Glaucoma Physician
  • New Retinal Physician
  • Ophthalmology Management
  • Ophthalmic Professional
  • Presbyopia Physician
  • Retinal Physician
The Ophthalmologist
  • Explore

    Explore

    • Latest
    • Insights
    • Case Studies
    • Opinion & Personal Narratives
    • Research & Innovations
    • Product Profiles

    Featured Topics

    • Anterior Segment
    • Glaucoma
    • Retina

    Issues

    • Latest Issue
    • Archive
  • Subspecialties
    • Cataract
    • Cornea
    • Glaucoma
    • Neuro-ophthalmology
    • Oculoplastics
    • Pediatric
    • Retina
  • Business

    Business & Profession

    • Professional Development
    • Business and Entrepreneurship
    • Practice Management
    • Health Economics & Policy
  • Training & Education

    Career Development

    • Professional Development
    • Career Pathways

    Events

    • Webinars
    • Live Events
  • Events
    • Live Events
    • Webinars
  • Community

    People & Profiles

    • Power List
    • Voices in the Community
    • Authors & Contributors
  • Multimedia
    • Video
    • Podcasts
Subscribe
Subscribe

False

Advertisement
The Ophthalmologist / Issues / 2025 / Jan / The Wolf and the Sheep
Research & Innovations Neuro-ophthalmology

The Wolf and the Sheep

Paranoid and teleological thinking is linked to distinct social hallucinations in vision, says new study

By The Ophthalmologist 1/21/2025 1 min read

Share

Credit: Wellcome Library, London. Wellcome Images images@wellcome.ac.uk, http://wellcomeimages.org Etching by W-S Howitt.

A recent Nature study has investigated how paranoia and teleological thinking – i.e., beliefs attributing malicious intent or excessive purpose to ordinary events – can manifest themselves in visual perception, revealing their distinct contributions to social hallucinations. Using dynamic visual stimuli, the research explored how individuals perceive agency, particularly the intention of one object "chasing" another.

In order to test how paranoia and teleology might hold their roots in visual perception, participants were asked to complete a series of experiments involving animated displays where discs mimicked predator-prey dynamics. In some trials, a "wolf" pursued a "sheep," while in others, no intentional chasing occurred. Researchers assessed participants’ ability to detect chasing, identify the two roles (wolf or sheep), and how confident they felt in the accuracy of their descriptions.

Using validated psychometric scales to establish participants who were prone to paranoia and/or teleological thinking, the researchers found that participants with high paranoia were more likely to perceive chasing in its absence, mistaking random motion as deliberate pursuit. Similarly, excessive teleological thinkers were shown to over-attribute purpose, and made similar errors to those with high paranoia, but with more distinct biases. Interestingly, high-paranoia individuals also struggled to correctly identify the “sheep,” which reflected their heightened sensitivity to perceived threats. In contrast the teleological thinkers had difficulty pinpointing the “wolf,” an omission which suggested an over-emphasis on these individuals on finding purpose rather than identifying any threats. The two groups both exhibited high confidence in their incorrect perceptions, which underscored the hallucinatory nature of these errors.

The findings offer new insights into how individuals can construct social meanings from ambiguous visual cues, and how high-level cognitive beliefs influence low-level perceptual processes, bridging the gap between vision science and mental health. The authors suggest that social hallucinations – i.e., errors in perceiving agency and intention – arise from the interplay of top-down beliefs and bottom-up sensory processing.

The insights could hold promise for further clinical application, with the authors noting that their present paradigm might provide clinicians with a simple visual test with which they can test which patients might benefit from interventions for paranoid thinking, as well as aiding in diagnosing or tailoring interventions for paranoia or related conditions, such as schizophrenia.

About the Author(s)

The Ophthalmologist

More Articles by The Ophthalmologist

Related Content

Newsletters

Receive the latest Ophthalmology news, personalities, education, and career development – weekly to your inbox.

Newsletter Signup Image

False

Advertisement

False

Advertisement

Explore More in Ophthalmology

Dive deeper into the world of Ophthalmology. Explore the latest articles, case studies, expert insights, and groundbreaking research.

False

Advertisement
The Ophthalmologist
Subscribe

About

  • About Us
  • Work at Conexiant Europe
  • Terms and Conditions
  • Privacy Policy
  • Advertise With Us
  • Contact Us

Copyright © 2025 Texere Publishing Limited (trading as Conexiant), with registered number 08113419 whose registered office is at Booths No. 1, Booths Park, Chelford Road, Knutsford, England, WA16 8GS.

Disclaimer

The Ophthalmologist website is intended solely for the eyes of healthcare professionals. Please confirm below: