Conexiant
Login
  • Corneal Physician
  • Glaucoma Physician
  • New Retinal Physician
  • Ophthalmology Management
  • Ophthalmic Professional
  • Presbyopia Physician
  • Retinal Physician
The Ophthalmologist
  • Explore

    Explore

    • Latest
    • Insights
    • Case Studies
    • Opinion & Personal Narratives
    • Research & Innovations
    • Product Profiles

    Featured Topics

    • Anterior Segment
    • Glaucoma
    • Retina

    Issues

    • Latest Issue
    • Archive
  • Subspecialties
    • Cataract
    • Cornea
    • Glaucoma
    • Neuro-ophthalmology
    • Oculoplastics
    • Pediatric
    • Retina
  • Business

    Business & Profession

    • Professional Development
    • Business and Entrepreneurship
    • Practice Management
    • Health Economics & Policy
  • Training & Education

    Career Development

    • Professional Development
    • Career Pathways

    Events

    • Webinars
    • Live Events
  • Events
    • Live Events
    • Webinars
  • Community

    People & Profiles

    • Power List
    • Voices in the Community
    • Authors & Contributors
  • Multimedia
    • Video
    • Podcasts
Subscribe
Subscribe

False

Advertisement
The Ophthalmologist / Issues / 2014 / Nov / Reading Scleral Signals

Reading Scleral Signals

The way that babies process information from eye expressions adds another piece to the social and emotional development puzzle

11/17/2014 1 min read

Share

1014-206-upfront-main

Eyes are important in social interactions. They’re the first and most influential feature we look at in other people’s faces (1), a critical component of reading others’ emotions (2), and instrumental in cooperative behavior (3). Human eyes, in particular, are unique in that the sclera is large and highly visible – and, because of this, it plays a big part in social and emotional cues. Wide eyes, for instance, indicate fear or surprise. We also use the sclera to determine where someone’s focus lies.

Our ability to read cues from the sclera is thought to facilitate many social and cooperative interactions, and is known to be hardwired into the adult human brain. Until now, though, it has been unclear whether or not the ability to read and process these cues exists in early development.

To find out, researchers from the Max Planck Institute in Germany and the University of Virginia in the US measured the brain responses of seven-month-old infants to a selection of different eye expressions (4). The infants were shown images of wide and narrowed eyes with direct and averted gazes while their brain activity was measured by electroencephalography. The images were shown for only 50 milliseconds – far less time than necessary to consciously perceive visual information. The infants in the study exhibited more brain activity in response to “fearful” eyes with visible sclera than to “non-fearful” eyes, and responded more to direct gazes than averted gazes. “This demonstrates that, like adults, infants are sensitive to eye expressions of fear,” says Tobias Grossmann, study co-author and director of the Early Social Development Group at the Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences. “Their brains clearly responded to social cues conveyed through the eyes, indicating that even without conscious awareness, human infants are able to detect subtle social cues.”

Though infants respond sensitively to eye cues from birth, characteristics such as eye orientation decline quickly in those later diagnosed with social deficits like autism. This study shows for the first time that responses such as fear and gaze detection occur subconsciously and that they are based on scleral signals only possible in human eyes. The existence of infants’ responses to eye cues, says Grossmann, “likely provides a vital foundation for the development of social interactive skills.”

References

  1. M. Bindemann, C. Scheepers and A. M. Burton, “Viewpoint and center of gravity affect eye movements to human faces”, J. Vis., 9, 1–16, (2009). doi: 10.1167/9.2.7. S. Baron-Cohen et al., “The ‘Reading the Mind in the Eyes’ test revised version: a study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism”, J. Child Psychol. Psychiat., 42, 241–251, (2001). M. Ernest-Jones, D. Nettle, M. Bateson, “Effects of eye images on everyday cooperative behavior: a field experiment”, Evol. Hum. Behav., 32, 172–178, (2011). S. Jessen, T. Grossmann, “Unconscious discrimination of social cues from eye whites in infants”, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. Epub ahead of print (2014).

Related Content

Newsletters

Receive the latest Ophthalmology news, personalities, education, and career development – weekly to your inbox.

Newsletter Signup Image

False

Advertisement

False

Advertisement

Explore More in Ophthalmology

Dive deeper into the world of Ophthalmology. Explore the latest articles, case studies, expert insights, and groundbreaking research.

False

Advertisement
The Ophthalmologist
Subscribe

About

  • About Us
  • Work at Conexiant Europe
  • Terms and Conditions
  • Privacy Policy
  • Advertise With Us
  • Contact Us

Copyright © 2025 Texere Publishing Limited (trading as Conexiant), with registered number 08113419 whose registered office is at Booths No. 1, Booths Park, Chelford Road, Knutsford, England, WA16 8GS.

Disclaimer

The Ophthalmologist website is intended solely for the eyes of healthcare professionals. Please confirm below: